67 pages • 2 hours read
Bill SchuttA modern alternative to SparkNotes and CliffsNotes, SuperSummary offers high-quality Study Guides with detailed chapter summaries and analysis of major themes, characters, and more.
Content Warning: This section contains references to illness and death.
In 1985, veterinarians in England were puzzled by cows exhibiting erratic behavior, loss of coordination, and an inability to stand. The affected animals were put down and processed into animal byproducts, but by 1986, the British Ministry of Agriculture launched an investigation. Scientists discovered that the cows’ brains were riddled with holes, similar to Swiss cheese, a characteristic of transmissible spongiform encephalopathies (TSEs), a group of neurological diseases that included scrapie in sheep and Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease (CJD) in humans. The newly identified disease in cows was named Bovine Spongiform Encephalopathy (BSE), or “Mad Cow Disease” (244). By 1987, cases had spread widely across England, and while government officials reassured the public that eating beef was safe, some scientists suspected a connection between BSE and kuru, a fatal disease once prevalent among the Fore people of New Guinea.
Kuru, first observed by Western researchers in the 1950s, was characterized by tremors, muscle weakness, loss of coordination, and pathological laughter, leading to death. It primarily affected women and children, and researchers soon linked its spread to ritual cannibalism, in which the Fore consumed the bodies of deceased relatives as a way of “honoring their dead” (246). The disease devastated the Fore population, killing about 1% of their members annually. Initially, Fore elders believed that kuru resulted from “sorcery” (247), and they retaliated by executing suspected sorcerers.
Early anthropological studies of the Fore, such as those by Ronald and Catherine Berndt in the 1950s, described widespread cannibalism but were criticized for exaggeration and sensationalism. More credible research came in the late 1950s when Daniel Carleton Gajdusek, an American virologist, began studying kuru. Gajdusek conducted fieldwork in New Guinea, collecting blood and tissue samples and interviewing locals. His findings, published in 1957, described kuru as “a degenerative disease of the central nervous system” (251) and noted that it affected more women than men, though children of both sexes were equally vulnerable. A hereditary explanation was initially considered but the disease’s spread over time contradicted a purely genetic cause.
Gajdusek’s team found that kuru-infected brains showed no signs of inflammation, ruling out bacterial or viral infections. Instead, researchers noted similarities between kuru and scrapie, a disease in sheep caused by an unknown agent. By the early 1960s, epidemiologists Robert and Shirley Glasse established a direct link between kuru and the Fore’s mortuary cannibalism, confirming that infected human tissues, particularly brains, transmitted the disease. Fore women and children, who prepared and consumed these tissues, were the most affected.
Further research revealed that kuru had emerged in the early 20th century and spread along kinship and trade networks in the New Guinea Highlands. The disease peaked in the late 1950s, with over 1,000 deaths, before declining in the 1960s and 1970s due to the prohibition of cannibalism. Though the epidemic was thought to be over, kuru’s long incubation period meant that isolated cases continued to appear for decades.
In the 1980s, British researchers, like their counterparts in New Guinea, investigated the origins and transmission of a deadly spongiform encephalopathy. Their breakthrough came when they linked the disease to the diet of dairy cows. In an effort to boost milk production, farmers were supplementing livestock feed with meat and bone meal, a processed mixture of animal byproducts, including bones, brains, spinal cords, and sick or dead animals. This practice, which began in the 1940s, unknowingly exposed cattle to infectious agents, much like the Fore people had been exposed to kuru through ritual cannibalism.
The sudden outbreak of Bovine Spongiform Encephalopathy (BSE) in the 1980s was traced to changes in the rendering process. Epidemiologist John Wilesmith discovered that in the early 1980s, rendering plants had stopped extracting tallow using solvents, a process that had inadvertently removed infectious agents. Additionally, meat and bone meal concentrations in livestock feed had increased significantly and calves were exposed to it at a younger age, shortening the disease’s incubation period. His 1988 study concluded that BSE had originated from contaminated sheep scrapie in livestock feed. However, his assumption that BSE and scrapie were identical led to dangerous consequences for British consumers in the years that followed.
Meanwhile, research into transmissible spongiform encephalopathies (TSEs) continued. In the 1940s, outbreaks of transmissible mink encephalopathy (TME) led scientists to suspect that infected feed containing sheep scrapie had transmitted the disease between species. By 1963, Daniel Carleton Gajdusek and NIH researcher Joe Gibbs conducted a groundbreaking experiment, inoculating chimpanzees with brain tissue from kuru victims. Within two years, the chimps exhibited symptoms identical to kuru, confirming that the disease was transmissible rather than genetic or psychological. The chimps’ condition deteriorated at a “frightening pace” (267).
Further research by Michael Alpers and Robert and Shirley Glasse strengthened the link between kuru and Fore mortuary cannibalism. By analyzing Fore genealogies, they noticed that kuru cases had started declining in children, coinciding with government efforts to ban ritual cannibalism in the 1950s. Since children had a shorter incubation period, the reduced consumption of infected tissue rapidly decreased the spread of kuru.
In 1966, Gajdusek, Gibbs, and Alpers published their findings in Nature, demonstrating the experimental transmission of kuru. However, Gajdusek initially hesitated to blame cannibalism, proposing instead that handling infected tissue, rather than consumption, had spread the disease. Over time, he conceded that both handling and ingestion could transmit kuru. Similar concerns about the spread of diseases through ritual handling of the dead persist today, particularly in Ebola outbreaks.
By the 1970s, interest in kuru research was “winding down” (271), but scrapie became a focus of investigation. South African biologist Tikvah Alper’s research showed that the scrapie agent was unlike any known virus: It lacked nucleic acids and was incredibly resistant to destruction. English mathematician J.S. Griffith proposed that the disease-causing agent was a self-replicating protein, an idea initially dismissed by the scientific community.
However, Stanley Prusiner at UC San Francisco pursued this idea, identifying an infectious protein that he named the prion, “an aberrant form of protein, which he claimed was responsible for the suite of neurodegenerative disorders known as Transmissible Spongiform Encephalopathies” (273). He argued that prions, unlike viruses, were misfolded proteins that could spread undetected by the immune system. In 1997, Prusiner won the Nobel Prize for his work, though critics accused him of overshadowing previous researchers and using unethical tactics to advance his career.
In 1988, despite epidemiologist John Wilesmith’s findings linking Bovine Spongiform Encephalopathy (BSE) to contaminated meat and bone meal, the British government “dragged its feet” (275). Concerned about economic consequences, officials avoided outright bans or public warnings, instead forming a panel led by zoologist Richard Southwood, who lacked experience with spongiform encephalopathies. A voluntary ban on ruminant-based protein feed was implemented, but with little enforcement, many farmers continued using the supplement to recover their financial investments.
Compensation for diseased cattle was set at only 50% of market value, incentivizing some farmers to send sick cows to slaughter rather than report them. This practice, though difficult to quantify, likely led to “significant” (277) human exposure to BSE-infected beef. The situation remained relatively hidden from the public until media coverage in 1988, including headlines like “Spongiform Fear Grows [and] Raging Cattle Attacks” (277), brought BSE to wider attention. A Nature paper further raised concerns by demonstrating that scrapie, a similar disease in sheep, could be transmitted to monkeys, lending weight to fears of cross-species infection.
The Southwood Committee’s 1989 report downplayed risks, asserting that human transmission of BSE was “remote” and unrelated to Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease (CJD). The government relied on these conclusions, projecting that BSE would fade by 1996 without further intervention. However, dissenting scientists, including Yale’s Dr. Laura Manuelidis, challenged the prevailing theory that prions—self-replicating misfolded proteins—were the cause of BSE and kuru. Instead, she argued that a still-unidentified virus was responsible for transmissible spongiform encephalopathies (TSEs), as misfolded proteins were found in various diseases without necessarily being infectious.
Public trust in British beef was further shaken in the 1990s. In 1990, Agriculture Minister John Gummer made an infamous televised attempt to reassure the public by feeding his four-year-old daughter a hamburger. However, troubling cases soon emerged. In 1993, two dairy farmers died of CJD, an unlikely statistical occurrence. Then in 1994, a 15-year-old girl named Victoria Rimmer exhibited kuru-like symptoms and a government investigator urged her family to remain silent to avoid damaging “the economy” (281). By 1995, several more young people were diagnosed with CJD, a disease previously rare in individuals under 30. In 1996, government advisor Dr. Eileen Rubery officially acknowledged that a new form of the disease, Variant Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease (vCJD), was transmitted via infected beef, marking the start of a human BSE epidemic.
By 2013, vCJD had caused 177 deaths in the UK. While some believed the epidemic was over, others feared that cases could continue appearing decades later, similar to kuru’s long incubation period. A study testing 32,000 appendix samples found that one in 2,000 UK residents carried the abnormal prion protein, suggesting widespread exposure. However, research by scientists like Simon Mead and John Collinge revealed a genetic variant in Fore survivors that conferred resistance to kuru, raising hope that some people exposed to prion-contaminated meat might never develop the disease. If Manuelidis’s viral theory proves correct, TSEs may be more mutable than previously believed, challenging the dominant prion hypothesis. The scientific debate continues, with implications for both disease prevention and public health policies.
Cannibalism, in an evolutionary sense, can be beneficial or detrimental depending on environmental conditions. In species like spiders, consuming excess males can improve reproductive success, but in populations where mates are scarce, it can reduce survival chances. This logical, predictable behavior contrasts with the moral and cultural complexities surrounding cannibalism in human societies.
Sigmund Freud theorized that “cultural or societal rules” (288) against cannibalism helped shape modern civilization, but historical evidence suggests that some cultures openly practiced it without the associated guilt. Societies such as the Fore of New Guinea and ancient Chinese communities incorporated cannibalism into rituals, sometimes as an act of respect. However, due to Western cultural influence, ritual cannibalism has likely disappeared from contemporary societies.
Despite its rarity, human fascination with cannibalism persists. This is evident in popular culture, from The Road to The Walking Dead, as well as in media sensationalism around cases like the “Cannibal Cop” Gilberto Valle. Cannibalistic crimes are often linked to psychological disorders, with experts suggesting that cannibal killers harbor deep-seated aggression. Yet, public intrigue fuels books, films, and sensational news coverage.
Some researchers argue that growing overpopulation and environmental stress could lead to widespread cannibalism, as it does among animals. Historical evidence shows that famine-induced cannibalism has occurred throughout history, particularly in times of crisis. Today, desertification and climate change threaten food and water supplies in regions like Africa and the Middle East, potentially setting the stage for future occurrences of cannibalism.
The dystopian film Soylent Green imagines “just such an environmental disaster scenario” (292) where overpopulation and resource depletion lead to government-sanctioned cannibalism. While fictional, this scenario raises real concerns about food scarcity. If mass starvation were to occur, historical patterns suggest that cannibalism could resurface as a survival strategy. Additionally, large-scale cannibalism could trigger outbreaks of diseases like kuru, a fatal neurological disorder previously linked to cannibalistic practices in New Guinea.
Anthropologist Simon Underdown proposes that “widespread practice of cannibalism and a kuru-like epidemic” (294) may have contributed to Neanderthal extinction. Small, isolated populations engaging in cannibalism might have unknowingly spread transmissible spongiform encephalopathies, gradually reducing their numbers. His model suggests that such a disease could have rendered Neanderthal populations non-viable within 250 years.
The ongoing scientific debate over whether TSEs are caused by prions or viruses remains unresolved. If future environmental crises lead to outbreaks of cannibalism, new neurological diseases could emerge, warranting further research. Given humanity’s history, it is likely that such a disease—should it arise—will be given a sensational name, “something lurid” (296), continuing society’s morbid fascination with cannibalism.
The closing chapters offer a global lens on cannibalism. The link between Kuru and Mad Cow Disease may appear tangential, but Schutt illustrates the way in which cultural taboos against cannibalism make discussions of the serious scientific implications of cannibalism a dangerous concern. By tying together the seemingly disparate topics, Schutt obliterates the cultural boundaries which might have delineated supposedly acceptable or taboo behavior. Though culturally distinct, the Fore and the British both fell victim to similar diseases. In this way, Schutt demonstrates why discussions of cannibalism are so important. They are not grounded in foreign, strange, or historically distant cultures. Rather, the issues deriving from cannibalism in its many forms are still ongoing and—from a scientific perspective—still grossly misunderstood. The mistakes among the anthropologists and the mistakes by the British scientists also illustrate the ways in which scientists are not beyond reproach. They suffer from presumptions and prejudices, often informed by cultural constructs like taboos, just as much as everyone else. The closing chapters of the book serve to illustrate that a better understanding of cannibalism is not only a global issue, but also one which applies to laymen and scientists alike. By demonstrating how both remote tribal practices and modern agricultural policies resulted in similar neurological consequences, Schutt invites readers to see cannibalism not as a fringe issue, but as a lens for understanding human fallibility, interconnectedness, and the costs of cultural denial.
The description of the BSE outbreak in Britain is one of the starkest warnings of the dangers of taboos in the book. Due to an aversion to the discussions of cannibalism, mistakes led to deaths and a great financial loss. This justifies Schutt’s desire to demystify the subject of cannibalism, but also shows the challenge goes beyond writing a book. The failures of the British government provide a template for Schutt’s argument, showing exactly how damaging the taboo against cannibalism can be, but also showing the power of the forces that propagate this taboo. This example highlights Media Sensationalism as a Problem, as public health was compromised by the government’s fear of reputational harm and economic panic, revealing how optics can override science. It also shows how cannibalism, as a concept, becomes a scapegoat—so culturally stigmatized that even scientific inquiry is distorted or avoided in the name of propriety. Schutt frames his work as a launch point for a broader conversation. The globalized nature of the matter furthers this framing, showing that people from across the world need to actively engage in the discussion of cannibalism to avoid similar tragedies and mistakes.
In his epilogue, Schutt looks to the future. The threat of climate change and the ongoing sensationalized nature of the media’s coverage of cannibalism presents a dual threat to civilization, he suggests. While the rest of the book engaged with the historical research into the past and present understanding of cannibalism, the epilogue looks forward. This shift in narrative tense is Schutt’s final argument in favor of his claim. The social taboo against cannibalism must be reexamined, he suggests, as the stakes may simply be too high. Here, Schutt returns to Breaking the Social Taboo of Cannibalism, framing it not just as an intellectual project but as a moral imperative in the face of global crisis. Bleak dystopian fiction about collapsing society is a warning against the way in which cannibalism may haunt the future without engaging with the subject in a sincere manner. Schutt’s look to the future abandons the wry, humorous tone of his earlier chapters: Schutt becomes serious and focused. This ominous warning urges readers to heed to Schutt’s advice and accept the perfectly natural status of cannibalism as a subject of discussion. In this way, the book closes not just as a history or scientific survey, but as a call to intellectual courage—urging readers to face discomfort for the sake of truth, public health, and shared humanity.
Animals in Literature
View Collection
Appearance Versus Reality
View Collection
Colonialism & Postcolonialism
View Collection
Community
View Collection
Globalization
View Collection
Mortality & Death
View Collection
Nature Versus Nurture
View Collection
Pride & Shame
View Collection
Safety & Danger
View Collection
Science & Nature
View Collection
The Future
View Collection
The Past
View Collection
Truth & Lies
View Collection
War
View Collection